Geographic Correlation and Causation

Correlation is a relation existing between phenomena or things or between mathematical or statistical variables which tend to vary, be associated, or occur together in a way not expected on the basis of chance alone. The analysis of correlation is one of the most common and useful tools in statistics. Accordingly, correlation is also a useful but dangerous tool in the analysis of spatial data.

Analyzing Correlation

Strength

Correlation is commonly visualized using an X/Y scatter chart, where one variable is plotted on the X axis and the other on the Y axis. If there is a correlation, the plotted points will form a clear pattern from left to right.

The strength of this correlation can be calculated as R2, the coefficient of determination. The range of R2 is from 0.000 (no correlation) to 1.000 (perfect correlation).

Exactly how R2 should be evaluated depends on the type of data being studied. In the natural sciences, values above 0.600 are often expected. However, in the social sciences where relationships often involve the complex interplay of ambiguous factors, values in the 0.200s or 0.300s can be considered meaningful for further investigation.

Strong Correlation

For example, there is a strong correlation between GDP per capita and energy consumption per capita by country. Residents in countries at a higher level of development use more energy for transportation, heating/cooling, food, appliances, etc.

Strong Geographic Correlation Between GDP Per Capita and Energy Use Per Capita (World Bank)

Weak Correlation

In some cases, the correlation is not quite as strong, meaning that there is a general upward or downward pattern on the X/Y scatter chart, but there are numerous outliers that are exceptions to the trend.

For example, the graph below shows that there is a weak correlation between GDP per capita and the adolescent fertility rate (births per 1,000 women ages 15-19 = teen pregnancy). While women in wealthy countries generally wait until their 20s or 30s to have their children, women in poorer countries often begin having children much earlier in life, reflecting characteristics like the traditions of that country, education levels for women, the ability of women to control their own destiny, etc.

However, there are a number of notable exceptions, such as the Sub-Saharan country of Burundi, which is poor but has an adolescent fertility rate similar to that of the wealthy United States.

Weak Geographic Correlation and Heteroskedasticity Between GDP Per Capita and Adolescent Fertility (World Bank)

No Correlation

When two variables with no correlation are plotted on the two axes of an X/Y scatter chart, the points form a diffuse cloud.

For example, there is no correlation between GDP per capita and the proportion of seats held by women in the national legislature. There are wealthy countries such as Sweden that have a high percentage of female legislators and there are poor countries like Rwanda that also have a high percentage of female legislators.

No Geographic Correlation Between GDP Per Capita and Percentage of Female Legislators (World Bank)

Positive vs. Negative Correlation

Positive Correlation

A positive correlation means that as one variable goes up, the other goes up as well. When two variables with a positive correlation are plotted on the two axes of an X/Y scatter chart, the points form a rough line or curve upward from left to right.

For example, in the aforementioned strong correlation between GDP per capita and energy use per capita, the correlation is positive:

Positive Geographic Correlation Between GDP Per Capita and Energy Use Per Capita (World Bank)

Negative Correlation

A negative correlation means that as one variable goes up, the other goes down. When two variables with a negative correlation are plotted on the two axes of an X/Y scatter chart, the points form a rough line or curve downward from left to right.

Using our prior example, there is a negative geographic correlation between GDP per capita and mortality of children under the age of five. Wealthier countries tend to have better nutrition, medical care, and social order than poorer countries. The wealthier the country, the lower the chance that a child will die before the age of five.

Negative Geographic Correlation Between GDP Per Capita and Mortality Before Age Five

Spatial vs. Temporal Correlation

The examples given above involve spatial correlation between values that represent multiple areas or locations at a single point in time.

Correlation can analyzed as temporal (time) correlation between variables measured in a single location at multiple points or time.

For example, in the United States, gross domestic product (adjusted for inflation) in each year historically has correlated strongly with electricity generation.

GDP vs. Electricity Generation (St. Louis Fed, EIA)

Empirical Limitations: Problems With Data

The use of simple correlation between geographic variables is a very useful tool for exploratory data analysis, but this technique has numerous weaknesses that require more-sophisticated statistical techniques to overcome.

The Modifiable Area Unit Problem

The modifiable area unit problem (MAUP) arises when working with data aggregated by areas or regions. If different area boundaries are used, that analysis can yield different results, even if the underlying phenomena is the same.

For example, county boundaries reflect historical and political processes rather than clear boundaries between areas where health outcomes are different. Therefore, use of these boundaries, which is often necessary to preserve confidentiality, limits the accuracy of analysis that is performed using data aggregated within those boundaries.

The Modifiable Areal Unit Problem

Autocorrelation

An issue related to the MAUP is that many human phenomena tend to correlate spatially with themselves. For example, areas of high income often cluster together as a reflection of a variety of social processes, including a desire by wealthy homeowners to maintain property values. That clustering can make observed correlations overestimate or underestimate the strength of the actual relationship between the phenomena under investigation.

Data Quality

Survey data subject to a number of issues that affect its accuracy. For example, areas with sparse populations limited medical facilities might underreport stroke diagnoses to researchers. People in some areas might also be suspicious, unresposive, or untruthful to researchers, resulting in less-accurate data.

Data quality can also be affected by the methodology by which it is created. In this example, data on fast food consumption is (apparently) modeled marketing research data which may reflect questionable modeling assumptions or limited availability of accurate data for model calibration.

Absolute vs Relative Values

When looking for correlations between characteristics in geographic area like countries that have a wide range of sizes, you will generally want to compare relative rather than absolute values.

Comparisons of countries using absolute values will often show correlation, but that correlation is between the size of the countries rather than the phenomena being analyzed. This is a specific case of confounding.

For example, when comparing absolute values of overall GNI (gross national income, or GDP plus income from overseas activities) to population, you see a strong correlation that implies that more-populous countries are also wealthier countries.

Correlation Between (Absolute) GDP and Population

However, that comparison does not consider how many people that wealth is distributed over. In analyzing the lives of people in a country, the overall wealth of the country is usually less important than how much of that wealth is available to individual people and families in that country.

If you compare relative values of GDP per capita with population, we see that there is no correlation. There are large countries where the residents (on average) are poor and small countries where the people (on average) are rich. The population of a country is not related to how wealthy individual people in a country are.

No Correlation Between (Relative) GDP per Capita and Population

Imperfect Regularities

Because causal relationships are often probabilistic and part of complex chains of causation, the absence of a correlation does not mean there is no causal relationship:

Non-linear Correlation

In the simplest forms of correlation, the relationship between the two variables forms a simple line. As one variable goes up one unit, the other variable goes up by one unit.

However, there are numerous instances where there is a relationship, but it forms a curve of some kind. For example, the relationship between stroke mortality by state and fruit consumption is non-linear. While stroke rates increase as fruit consumption drops, the effect is more extreme in states with unusually high stroke rates. In this case, a polynomial fits the pattern better than a straight line.

2017 Stroke Mortality vs Fruit Consumption by State (CDC)

Probabilistic Causation

In some simple situations, the effect resulting from a cause is immediate and clear, such as dropping a ball (cause) resulting in the ball falling to the ground (effect). These are called deterministic causal relationships. The cause always determines the effect and a correlation between cause and effect is readily observable as correlation.

However, in most real-world situations, especially those involving people, effects have multiple causes, causes do not always result in effects, and/or effects follow causes by varying lengths of time (if at all). Causes increase the probability of effects, so these are called probabilistic causal relationships.

Deterministic vs. Probabilistic Causation

As a geographic example: Proximity to a former industrial site (brownfield) that is contaminated with carcinogenic polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons correlates with increased rates of cancer for people that live close to that site. However, there are people who live close to that site that do not get cancer, and there are people who do not live close to any source of that carcinogen who will get that same type of cancer. In addition, carcinogens often take years to show their negative affects, and different genetic characteristics make some people more vulnerable than others to the effects of that carcinogen.

Techniques more sophisticated than simple correlation are needed for for clearly identifying probabilistic causes and separating all the myriad factors that could also be causes. For example, the Neyman-Rubin causal model involves finding pairs of people that are identical by all considered characteristics other than the characteristic that is suspected of being a cause, and observing them over time to see if the characteristic of interest affects the probability of the studied effect.

Temporal Lag

Causes may take time to manifest as an effect. This results in a delay (temporal lag) between one variable and another.

For example, stroke can be the result of years of unhealthy lifestyle choices that may have been made in locations other than the home of the stroke victim at the time of onset. Indeed, a stroke victim might be making very different lifestyle choices in retirement than they made when they were younger and began accumulating comorbidities. Accordingly, there may be a temporal (time) lag between data that would reflect the risk factors and the data that measures stroke. Such temporal lag can hide clear connections between risk factors and stroke incidence.

Spatial Lag

Human mobility is also a problem. Because people usually change locations during the day (home, work, school) and occasionally relocate residences, there may be a spatial lag between data reflecting risk factors and data on where people are when they have strokes.

Causes may result in effects that occur in different geographical areas. For example: A West Nile virus infection that was contracted from a mosquito bite in a recreational area may be diagnosed in a separate residential area (spatial lag)

Logical Limitations: Problems Of Interpretation

Causation refers to a cause-and-effect relationship between to things. Correlation can be used to find causal relationships, but causation is a more complex concept that a simple binary relationship. This tutorial introduces correlation and issues that distinguish correlation from causation.

While correlation may be interesting, what we are usually more interested in is causation - whether one of the phenomena we are observing causes the other phenomena. In this example, we are interested in whether being close to a natural-gas-fired power plant causes cancer. Such knowledge could be used to justify tighter emissions regulations for power plants, or to help promote public funding to accelerate alternative energy research and deployment.

While correlation is empirical (based on observation), causation is based on reason. When we observe two phenomena occurring together and we observe that there is some mechanism connecting the two phenomena, we use reason and logic to tie those two phenomena together in a cause and effect relationship.

Assuming that correlation proves causation is the post hoc fallacy, from the Latin phrase post hoc ergo propter hoc (after this, therefore because of this). A logical fallacy is an often plausible argument using false or invalid inference.

Correlation points to possible causal relationships, but does not prove them, and there are a variety of logical arguments to show how making a simple assumption that correlation is causation will lead you astray.

Covariates

Covariates are additional independent variables that might influence the dependent variable.

For example, stroke risk correlates smoking rates, but stroke risk also correlates with a wide range of other factors like diet, high blood pressure, family history, that also correlate with each other. Separating out how much of the increased stroke risk is due to smoking and how much is due to those other factors requires more complex techniques like regression analysis, principal component analysis, factor analysis.

Covariates

Confounders: The Third Variable Problem

Two variables that correlate with each other may individually have causation rooted in a third variable. In complex systems, these indirect relationships are often hard to identify and are referred to as confounding.

For example, per capita health expenditures is strongly correlated with per capita electricity use. In this case the electricity use is not caused by health expenditures or vice versa. Both of those two variables are correlated with economic development - a third variable which is more-reflective of the causes of increase electricity use and increased health care expenditures.

The Third Variable Problem

Asymmetry: Which is the Cause and Which is the Effect?

Reverse causation is when the assumed cause in a cause-and-effect relationship is actually the effect. For example, if a recreational drug user also has symptoms of mental illness, the assumption that the drug use is causing the mental illness may be reversed - the person may be self-medicating to attempt to ameliorate the symptoms of pre-existing mental illness.

Simultaneity occurs when cause and effect occur together, often in a feedback loop. For example, children from wealthy families often have opportunities to get into more-presitgious schools which improves their chances of being wealthy and passing on those advantages to their own children. Wealth causes improved educational opportunity which causes wealth.

In this example, the relationship between stroke and fast food consumption may actually be reversed. Stroke victims may be wary of continuing the lifestyle choices that led to their strokes, and may eschew fast food, lowering market demand.

Correlation shows the regularity of the relationship between two variables, but does not indicate which is the cause and which is the effect. This can often be seen as the classic chicken and egg problem: which came first, the chicken or the egg?

Using the correlation between GDP per capita and childhood death as an example, does poverty cause childhood death or does childhood death cause poverty? Societies respond to high levels of childhood death by having more children to increase the chances of having some children survive, and those high birth rates place burdens on families that prevent more-active participation in economic growth. In such a case, we could say that wealth and childhood survival are mutually constitutive.

Mutual Causation

Irrelevance: Spurious Correlations

There are numerous instances where correlations between variables are clearly either accidents or have a root cause that is so indirect as to be meaningless. Indeed, Tyler Vigen has devoted a website to celebrating these statistical oddities.

Spurious Correlations

Chains of Causation

Effects are commonly the result of chains of causation. The causes most directly associated with the effect are called proximate causes while the causes further up the chain are called ultimate causes or root causes.

Since correlations are simple comparison of two values, correlation will not identify these chains of causation, often leading to an oversimplified understanding of what exactly is going on.

For example:

In cases like this, correlations may provide evidence for proximate causes, but may not identify what are often more important ultimate causes that need to be addressed to solve problematic effects further down the chain of causation.

Example Causal Chain

The Ecological Fallacy

The ecological fallacy is making an assumption about individuals in an area based on aggregated statistics. For example, just because the median household income in a neighborhood is high does not mean that every individual person in that neighborhood lives in a high-income household.

Accordingly, making assumptions about causation in individual situations based on geographic correlation is fallacious. While areas that have above average numbers of smokers also tend to have high levels of stroke mortality, there are smokers that never have strokes, and people who have strokes who have never smoked.

Example Ecological Fallscy

The Problem of Induction

By offering an explanation in the form of a cause and effect relationship, we are making an inference that when that cause or set of causes exists, the effect will occur or has an increased probability of occurring.

But while correlation can tell us that this was true in a specific set of places in the past, that does not offer any logical proof that has to continue to be true in the future or in a different set of locations, since we cannot directly observe all possible locations in all possible futures.

The Problem of Induction

This is the problem of induction identified by the Scottish philosopher David Hume (1711 - 1776).

David Hume by Allan Ramsay (Wikimedia)

This is similar to the statement, you cannot prove a negative. While you can offer empirical evidence from the past that something has never happened, you cannot offer direct evidence that something will never happen in a future you haven't experienced yet.

One way to mitigate this problem is the concept of falsification proposed by the Austrian philosopher Karl Popper (1902 - 1994).

While you cannot prove a hypothesis will always be true, you only have to find one situation where a hypothesis is false to show that it is not true and needs to be modified or abandoned. If you fail to falsify a theory, you corroborate that theory. Corroboration does not prove a hypothesis is true, but multiple corroborations increase your confidence that your theory may be applicable in other times and places. This approach is the basis of the concept of the null hypothesis used in inferential statistics.

Why Does Causation Matter?

Critical Thinking

In contemporary life we are constantly bombarded with numerous streams of narratives and contradictory information. Sorting out these narratives and recognizing fallacies is easier when you have a clearer understanding of what causation is and how it can be evaluated.

The post hoc fallacy is dangerous because it can lead to falsely assigning credit (or blame) for a positive (or negative) effect and lead to unnecessary, misleading, or dangerous actions that will do little to achieve the desired outcomes, and might even be counterproductive.

For example, there is a strong negative correlation between fruit consumption and lowered risk of stroke mortality by US state. Interpreting this correlation by saying that low fruit consumption causes stroke death would fall into the post hoc fallacy. If you take this further and assume that all that needs to be done is encourage more fruit consumption while ignoring all the other risk factors (smoking, sedentary lifestyles, high consumption of sodum and fat, family history, etc.), at best you probably would be wasting your effort, and, at worst, you would be distracting people from making the lifestyle changes that would more-significantly reduce their chances of dying from a stroke.

2017 Stroke Mortality vs Fruit Consumption by State (CDC)

Liability

In many areas of life like management and law, determining responsibility for actions and activities is important to hold members of an organization or society accountable, and promote justice.

Forecasting and Planning

Understanding the causes of the effects that impact our lives can help us anticipate what those effects could be in the future. This can aid us both collectively and individually in planning for possible futures.

Intervention

Understanding causation can often allow us to intervene and prevent undesirable effects or promote desirable effects. These interventions can be at the individual level, up to the level of public policy. For example, in health, understanding the etiology of a disease is essential for determining how that disease can be prevented or mitigated.